News

Medical Students' Biases Don't Affect Clinical Decisions

View on the News

Why Bias May Affect Students Differently

The reason that medical students didn’t demonstrate their mostly unconscious racial and class biases in this study may be that they were able to focus their attention fully on the hypothetical vignettes and were motivated to get the "right" answer. In contrast, physicians in practice make clinical decisions in an atmosphere characterized by stress, multiple demands on working memory, distractions, a heavy workload, and time pressure, said Michelle van Ryn, Ph.D., and Dr. Somnath Saha.

Decision making is less likely to be affected by unconscious bias "when people are engaging in deliberate, thought-out decisions and have the cognitive resources, motivation, and opportunity to consider the pros and cons of different actions." In contrast, bias may take precedence when cognitive processing capacity is taxed and "people are less able to override automatic categorizing and stereotyping," they noted.

"When clinicians have sufficient cognitive resources, time, information, and motivation to be unbiased, their intent to provide equitable care is not undermined by biased implicit attitudes," Dr. van Ryn and Dr. Saha said.

Dr. van Ryn is in the department of family medicine and community health at the University of Minnesota, Minneapolis. Dr. Saha is in internal medicine at Portland Veterans Affairs Medical Center and Oregon Health & Science University, Portland. They reported no financial conflict of interest. These remarks were taken from their editorial (JAMA 2011;306:995-6) that accompanied the report by Dr. Haider and his colleagues.


 

FROM JAMA

First-year medical students show an unconscious preference toward the white race and the upper class on two tests that measure automatic responses to racial and social visual cues, according to a report in the Sept. 7 issue of JAMA.

But surprisingly, these preferences did not influence the students’ clinical assessments or decision making in a series of hypothetical medical vignettes, said the researchers, led by Dr. Adil H. Haider, codirector of the Center for Surgical Trials and Outcomes Research at the Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore.

Previous studies among older physicians have shown that such unconscious biases influence their clinical decisions, the researchers noted. For example, physicians typically prescribe fewer analgesics for black or Hispanic patients than they do for white patients, despite identical self-reported assessments of pain. In turn, these implicit preferences could be the source of some of the race- and class-based disparities in American health care, the team said.

"Unconscious biases are normal and rooted in stereotyping, a cognitive process in which individuals use a social category to acquire, process, and recall information about people," Dr. Haider and his colleagues explained.

To assess unconscious race and class biases, the team did a Web-based survey of two cohorts of 202 first-year medical students at a single institution. The study subjects responded to eight written clinical vignettes that involved assessing pain, obtaining consent for a procedure, judging the reliability of a patient or family, and assessing the trustworthiness of hypothetical patients representing two races (black and white) and two social classes (upper and lower class).

The students also took the validated, race-focused Implicit Association Test (IAT), which assesses unconscious attitudes toward racial groups by measuring reaction times to visual and language cues, and a similar but not yet validated social class–focused IAT. The study subjects also answered direct questions about their explicit racial and class biases and provided demographic information, including their own ethnicity.

As in the general population, most of the medical students reported that they had no conscious racial preferences. Only 39% said they explicitly preferred white people and 7% said they explicitly preferred black people. The subjects appeared less guarded in their self-reports of class bias, with 51% saying they explicitly preferred upper-class individuals and 12% saying they explicitly preferred lower-class individuals.

However, the IATs revealed that these conscious preferences did not correlate well with subjects’ implicit (unconscious) preferences. The IAT results showed that unconsciously, 69% of medical students preferred white people and 86% preferred upper-class individuals, which are numbers similar to those in studies of the general population.

Yet, the medical students’ responses to the medical vignettes did not reflect either their unconscious or conscious preferences. There was no association between the hypothetical patients’ race or class and the students’ responses to the clinical situations. Similarly, there was no association between the hypothetical patients’ race or class and the students’ IAT scores, the investigators said (JAMA 2011;306:942-51).

In further analyses, the researchers continued to find no impact of the students’ biases on clinical decision making. These analyses included categorizing the students’ clinical responses according to their races and self-reported social classes. Even in the subgroup of students who admitted to having explicit racial (93 students) and class (125 students) biases, no clinical–decision making effect appeared.

The findings among medical students stand in stark contrast to results among practicing physicians, where "implicit race and social class biases ... are increasingly recognized as potential factors contributing to disparities in health care," Dr. Haider and his colleagues said.

They deemed the reasons for this discrepancy between students and physicians unknown. Possibly, because the students are younger, they "have been more exposed to educational curricula focused on cultural competency, translating to improved awareness and management of implicit bias," the team said.

It also is possible that students who "have not yet been exposed to the rigors of medical training" might not yet be influenced by implicit biases, the researchers suggested.

They called for further study of whether experiences during medical training influence social or racial bias in clinical decision making. If so, "medical training could be an effective intervention point to decrease implicit biases and possibly mitigate physician-driven health care disparities," they added.

The study was supported by the National Institute of General Medical Sciences, the National Institutes of Health, the American College of Surgeons, and the National Heart, Lung, and Blood Institute. The authors reported no financial conflict of interest.

Recommended Reading

Federal Court Rules Health Law's Mandate Unconstitutional
MDedge Dermatology
U.S. Dermatology Procedures Doubled From 1995-2008
MDedge Dermatology
Dermatology CME Focuses on Performance Improvement
MDedge Dermatology
Nearly All Physicians Eventually Face a Malpractice Claim
MDedge Dermatology
Report: Trend of Hospital-Employed Docs Could Raise Costs
MDedge Dermatology
Medicare to Begin Testing Bundled Payments
MDedge Dermatology
Health Care Access: Survey Shows Recession's Effects
MDedge Dermatology
Dermatologists' Income Up 2% in 2010
MDedge Dermatology
Post-Anthrax Public Health Advances Threatened
MDedge Dermatology
CMS Eases Requirements for E-Prescribing
MDedge Dermatology