Ninety-four of the papers reviewed (78%) referred to the Kitahata et al study as evidence of the association between experience and patient outcomes (“experience articles”). Ten of the papers (8%) referred to the study as evidence of the benefits of specialty or specialized care (“specialty articles”); quotations from those papers are shown in Table 2.6-15 Four of the papers reviewed (3%) referred to the study as evidence of the benefits of expertise or expert care (“expert articles”); these quotations are shown in Table 3.16-19 Thirteen of the articles reviewed (11%) listed the study as a general reference or cited it for reasons other than making an assertion about the relationship between health care provider characteristics and patient outcomes. On the initial review, the authors’ classification of the passage differed for only 5 (4%) of the 121 articles reviewed; all of these were ultimately classified as “experience” or “other” articles.
“Specialty articles” were more likely to be non-HIV related (80%) than HIV related, while “experience articles” were more likely to be HIV related (81%); this difference was statistically significant (P <.001). “Specialty articles” were also more likely to be reviews or editorials (80%); most of the “experience articles” were original research (59%); the P value for this difference was 0.02 by Fisher’s exact test.
Discussion
This study illustrates the various ways in which the results of a single study are interpreted and conveyed to readers of scientific papers. The study by Kitahata and colleagues was a comparison of generalists with varying levels of experience. It was not a study of specialists or specialized care; nevertheless, it was presented as such in 8% of the articles reviewed. Another 3% cited the study as evidence of the benefits of “expert care”; this is perhaps justifiable, but the study did not measure expertise, and the leap from experience to expertise is questionable, at best.
There are a few possible explanations for the authors’ misrepresentation of the article by Kitahata et al. The most obvious explanation is that the authors in these situations were willing to manipulate the results of the study to bolster their argument. Most of the articles that referred to this study as evidence of the benefits of specialized care (8 of 10) were review articles on the topic of specialized care815 and half of those were focused on hospitalism9,11-13; these are settings in which the authors may have had an incentive to present the study in this manner. It is possible, however, that the authors felt free to extrapolate from the results of this study and argue that since a relatively modest increase in experience improves outcomes, an even greater increase (ie, specialization) would improve outcomes even more. However, this argument was never explicitly made in these examples. A few of the articles reviewed did make this type of argument, but we classified these as “experience” articles. A final possible explanation is that the authors used a previous author’s reference without reviewing the study themselves; however, this seems less likely, since the title of the study clearly states that physician experience was the variable studied.
Limitations
Our study has a number of limitations. The source article was not chosen randomly, but was based on an observation of misrepresentation, so the frequency of misuse may be higher than with other articles. However, the misquotation rate found in this study is consistent with previous studies. One analysis of 6 journals reported a misquotation rate of 15%; 8% of these were felt to be major errors.3 A second study found 37 major quotation errors in 150 randomly selected references from 137 different articles.4 Another limitation of this study is that not all the articles that cited the study were analyzed; however, the number of unanalyzed studies is fairly small (8%) and unlikely to affect the overall conclusions.
Conclusions
Authors of scientific papers have a responsibility to convey accurately the information they have gathered to their readers. This study suggests that some authors are willing to bend or break this rule. It is unlikely that reminders about adherence to the responsibilities of authorship will alter this habit. Increased scrutiny of references in the peer-review process, although difficult, is most likely the only way to guard against these types of manipulations and misrepresentations.